Sobre la reciprocidad de naturaleza y cultura. La Cognición 4E y su perspectiva arqueológica en el paisaje
Resumen En el ámbito arqueológico, una de las ultimas fronteras de conocimiento se ha situado en el entendimiento de la mente antigua a través de sus restos materiales. En las últimas décadas, aprovechando las innovaciones técnicas y epistemológicas, se ha abierto un nuevo abanico de posibilidades para este tipo de estudios. La llamada arqueología cognitiva nace para resolver el problema paradigmático entre naturaleza y cultura y para intentar estudiar los procesos cognitivos de las sociedades pasadas en la creación y desarrollo cultural que se dio en ellas. El presente estudio pretende poner en valor dicha especialidad arqueológica, haciendo un repaso de su historiografía y poniendo de relevancia uno de los campos de estudio de esta especialidad que es la llamada cognición 4E. Por último, proponemos la inclusión del paisaje como un elemento fundamental de estudio dentro de esta especialidad, por su influencia y rol en el desarrollo cultural, como han demostrado estudios muy recientes. Una aproximación donde las barreras entre ciencias exactas y sociales debe romperse para obtener un mejor entendimiento de la cultura como totalidad.
- Referencias
- Cómo citar
- Del mismo autor
- Métricas
Agre, P. E., y Chapman, D. (1987). Pengi: An implementation of a theory of activity. En K. Forbus y H. Shrobe (eds.), Proceedings of the sixth National conference on Artificial intelligence—Volume 1 (pp. 268-272). Huntsville, Ala.: AAAI Press.
Antonson, H., Mårdh, S., Wiklund, M., y Blomqvist, G. (2009). Effect of surrounding landscape on driving behaviour: A driving simulator study. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(4), pp. 493-502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.03.005
Arnheim, R. (2002). Arte y percepción visual: Psicología del ojo creador. Madrid: Alianza.
Baltes, P. B., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., y Rösler, F. (2006). Lifespan development and the brain: The perspective of biocultural co-constructivism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511499722
Barnard, P. J. (2010). From Executive Mechanisms Underlying Perception and Action to the Parallel Processing of Meaning. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S39-S54. https://doi.org/10.1086/650695
Barnard, P. J., Duke, D. J., Byrne, R. W., y Davidson, I. (2007). Differentiation in cognitive and emotional meanings: An evolutionary analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 21(6), 1155-1183. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701437477
Barona, A. M., y Malafouris, L. (2021). On making futures with human touch. Adaptive Behavior, 30(6), 551-553. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059712321989428
Barrett, J. C. (2012). Agency: A revisionist account. En I. Hodder (ed.), Archaeological Theory Today (pp. 146-166). Cambridge: Polity.
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded Cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 617-645. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
Bateson, G. (2008). Steps to an ecology of mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Beja-Pereira, A., Luikart, G., England, P. R., Bradley, D. G., Jann, O. C., Bertorelle, G., Chamberlain, A. T., Nunes, T. P., Metodiev, S., Ferrand, N., y Erhardt, G. (2003). Gene-culture coevolution between cattle milk protein genes and human lactase genes. Nature Genetics, 35(4), 311-313. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng1263
Benazzi, S., Douka, K., Fornai, C., Bauer, C. C., Kullmer, O., Svoboda, J., Pap, I., Mallegni, F., Bayle, P., Coquerelle, M., Condemi, S., Ronchitelli, A., Harvati, K., y Weber, G. W. (2011). Early dispersal of modern humans in Europe and implications for Neanderthal behaviour. Nature, 479(7374), Article 7374. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10617
Bender, A. (2019). The role of culture and evolution for human cognition. Top. Cogn. Sci. 12 (4), pp. 1403-1420. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12449
Bender, A. (2020). What Is Causal Cognition?. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00003
Bender, A., Beller, S. y Medin, D. L. (2017). Causal Cognition and Culture. En Michael R. Waldmann (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Causal Reasoning (pp. 717-738). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199399550.013.34
Bercury, K. K. and Macklin, W. B. (2015). Dynamics and Mechanisms of CNS Myelination. Developmental Cell, 32(4), pp. 447-458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.devcel.2015.01.016
Berto, R., Massaccesi, S., y Pasini, M. (2008). Do eye movements measured across high and low fascination photographs differ? Addressing Kaplan’s fascination hypothesis. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(2), pp. 185-191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.11.004
Białowąs, S., y Szyszka, A. (2019). Eye-tracking in Marketing Research. En R. Romanowski (ed.), Managing Economic Innovations – Methods and Instruments (pp. 91-104). Poznań: Bogucki Wyd. https://doi.org/10.12657/9788379862771-6
Binford, S. R., y Binford, L. R. (1968). New perspectives in archeology. Chicago: Aldine Pub. Co.
Bjorklund, D., y Sellers, P. (2014). Memory Development in Evolutionary Perspective. En P. J. Bauer y R. Fivush (eds.), The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory (Vol.1, pp. 126-156). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118597705.ch7
Boivin, N. (2004). Mind over matter? Collapsing the mind-matter dichotomy in material culture studies. En E. DeMarris, C. Gosden, y C. Renfrew (eds.), Rethinking materiality: The engagement of mind with the material world (pp. 63-71). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.
Bruner, E. (2010). Morphological Differences in the Parietal Lobes within the Human Genus: A Neurofunctional Perspective. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S77-S88. https://doi.org/10.1086/650729
Byrge, L., Sporns, O., y Smith, L. B. (2014). Developmental process emerges from extended brain-body-behavior networks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), pp. 395-403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.010
Campanaro, D. M., y Landeschi, G. (2022). Re-viewing Pompeian domestic space through combined virtual reality-based eye tracking and 3D GIS. Antiquity, 96 (386), pp. 479-486. https://doi.org/10.15184/aqy.2022.12
Chen, C., Burton, M., Greenberger, E., y Dmitrieva, J. (1999). Population Migration and the Variation of Dopamine D4 Receptor (DRD4) Allele Frequencies Around the Globe. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(5), pp. 309-324. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00015-X
Chiao, J. Y., y Blizinsky, K. D. (2010). Culture-gene coevolution of individualism-collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 277(1681), pp. 529-537. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1650
Chiao, J. Y., Cheon, B. K., Pornpattanangkul, N., Mrazek, A. J., y Blizinsky, K. D. (2013). Cultural Neuroscience: Progress and Promise. Psychological inquiry, 24(1), pp. 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2013.752715
Chiao, J. Y., Harada, T., Oby, E. R., Li, Z., Parrish, T., y Bridge, D. J. (2009). Neural representations of social status hierarchy in human inferior parietal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), pp. 354-363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.023
Chiao, J. Y., Hariri, A. R., Harada, T., Mano, Y., Sadato, N., Parrish, T. B., y Iidaka, T. (2010). Theory and methods in cultural neuroscience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2-3), pp. 356-361. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq063
Chiao, J. Y., Iidaka, T., Gordon, H. L., Nogawa, J., Bar, M., Aminoff, E., Sadato, N., y Ambady, N. (2008). Cultural specificity in amygdala response to fear faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(12), pp. 2167-2174. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20151
Chiao, J. Y., Zhang, L., y Sadato, N. (2022). Culture and Genomics. En J. Y. Chiao, S. C. Li, R. Turner, S. Y. Lee-Tauler, y B. Pringle (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cultural Neuroscience and Global Mental Health (pp. 144-155). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190057695.013.7
Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E., y Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(35), pp. 12629-12633. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506162102
Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/1552.001.0001
Clark, A. (2006). Material symbols. Philosophical Psychology, 19(3), pp. 291-307. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515080600689872
Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195333213.001.0001
Clark, A. (2010). Material surrogacy and the Supernatural: Reflections on the Role of Artefacts in «Off-line» cognition. En L. Malafouris, C. Renfrew, y McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research (eds.), The cognitive life of things: Recasting the boundaries of the mind (pp. 23-28). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research ; Distributed by Oxbow Books ; USA [distributor], David Brown Co.
Clark, A. (2023). The experience machine: How our Minds Predict and Shape Reality. Londres: Allen Lane.
Clark, A., y Chalmers, D. (1998). The Extended Mind. Analysis, 58(1), pp. 7-19. https://doi.org/10.1093/analys/58.1.7
Clarke, D. L. (1968). Analytical archaeology. Londres: Methuen.
Colagè, I. y d’Errico, F. (2018). Culture: The Driving Force of Human Cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(12), pp. 1-19. En Bender, A., Beller, S. and Jordan, F. (Topic Editors). The Cultural Evolution of Cognition. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12372
Cole, M. (1974). The Cultural context of learning and thinking: An exploration in experimental anthropology. Londres: Tavistock Pub.
Constant, A., Tschantz, A. D. D., Millidge, B., Criado-Boado, F., Martinez, L. M., Müeller, J., y Clark, A. (2021). The acquisition of culturally patterned attention styles under active inference. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 15, a729665. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbot.2021.729665
Cook, A. (2018). 4E cognition and the humanities. En A. Newen, L. de Bruin, y S. Gallagher (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition (pp. 875-890). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.47
Coolidge, F. L., y Wynn, T. (2005). Working Memory, its Executive Functions, and the Emergence of Modern Thinking. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 15(1), pp. 5-26. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774305000016
Costello, M. C., y Bloesch, E. K. (2017). Are Older Adults Less Embodied? A Review of Age Effects through the Lens of Embodied Cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00267
Criado Boado, F., Alonso-Pablos, D., Blanco, M. J., Porto, Y., Rodríguez-Paz, A., Cabrejas, E., del Barrio-Álvarez, E., y Martínez, L. M. (2019). Coevolution of visual behaviour, the material world and social complexity, depicted by the eye-tracking of archaeological objects in humans. Scientific Reports, 9(1), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-39661-w
Criado-Boado, F., Martínez, L. M., Blanco, M. J., Alonso-Pablos, D., y Verdonkschot, J. (2024). Archaeologiques of sight: The visual world fosters the engagement between doing, seeing, and thinking. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 73, 101568. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2023.101568
Damasio, A. R. (2000). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Londres: William Heinemann.
Davidson, I., y Noble, W. (1989). The Archaeology of Perception: Traces of Depiction and Language [and Comments and Reply]. Current Anthropology, 30(2), pp. 125-155. https://doi.org/10.1086/203723
De Lucio, J. V., Mohamadian, M., Ruiz, J. P., Banayas, J., y Bernaldez, F. G. (1996). Visual landscape exploration as revealed by eye movement tracking. Landscape and Urban Planning, 34(2), pp. 135-142. https://doi.org/10.1016/0169-2046(95)00208-1
Dehaene, S. y Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56, pp. 384-398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.004
D’Errico, F. (2003). The invisible frontier. A multiple species model for the origin of behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(4), pp. 188-202. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.10113
D’Errico, F., y Stringer, C. B. (2011). Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), pp. 1060-1069. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0340
Descola, P. (2013). Más allá de naturaleza y cultura. Buenos Aires: Amorrortu.
Domínguez, J. F., Lewis, E. D., Turner, R., y Egan, G. F. (2009). The brain in culture and culture in the brain: A review of core issues in neuroanthropology. Progress in Brain Research, 178, pp. 43-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17804-4
Domínguez, J. F., Turner, R., Lewis, E. D., y Egan, G. (2010). Neuroanthropology: A humanistic science for the study of the culture-brain nexus. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2-3), pp. 138-147. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp024
Donald, M. (1998). Material Culture and Cognition: Concluding Thoughts. En C. Renfrew y C. Scarre (eds.), Cognition and Material Culture: The Archaeology of Symbolic Storage (pp. 181-187). Cambridge: McDonald Institute Monographs.
Dupont, L., Antrop, M., y Van Eetvelde, V. (2013). Eye-tracking Analysis in Landscape Perception Research: Influence of Photograph Properties and Landscape Characteristics. Landscape Research, 39(4), pp. 417-432. https://doi.org/10.1080/01426397.2013.773966
Elfenbein, H. A., y Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), pp. 203-235. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203
Flor, N., y Hutchins, E. (1991). Analyzing distributed cognition in software teams: A case study of team programming during perfective software maintenance. En J. Koenemann-Belliveau, T. G. Moher, y S. Robertson (eds.), Proceedings of the Fourth Annual Workshop on empirical studies of programmers (pp. 36-59). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.
Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/0199271941.001.0001
Gallagher, S. (2013). The socially extended mind. Cognitive Systems Research, 25-26, pp. 4-12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2013.03.008
Gallagher, S. (2018). Situating Interaction in Peripersonal and Extrapersonal Space: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives. En T. Hünefeldt y A. Schlitte (eds.), Situatedness and Place: Multidisciplinary Perspectives on the Spatio-temporal Contingency of Human Life (pp. 67-79). Cham: Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-92937-8_5
García-Porrero, J. A. (1999). Evolución del cerebro: la génesis de la mente. In García-Porrero, J. A. (ed.), Genes, cultura y mente: una reflexión multidisciplinar sobre la naturaleza humana en la década del cerebro (pp. 129-158). Santander: Servicio de publicaciones de la Universidad de Cantabria.
Garofoli, D. (2015). Cognitive archaeology without behavioral modernity: An eliminativist attempt. Quaternary International, 405, pp. 125-135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.06.061
Garofoli, D. (2016). Metaplasticit-ies: Material Engagement Meets Mutational Enhancement. En G. Etzelmüller y C. Tewes (Eds.), Embodiment in Evolution and Culture (pp. 307-336). Aufl. Tubingen: Mohr Siebeck GmbH and Co. KG. https://www.jstor.org/stable/j.ctt2250vc6.22
Garofoli, D., y Haidle, M. N. (2014). Epistemological problems in Cognitive Archaeology: An anti-relativistic proposal towards methodological uniformity. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 92, pp. 7-41. https://doi.org/10.4436/jass.91003
Gendron, M., Mesquita, B., y Barrett, L. F. (2020). The Brain as a Cultural Artifact: Concepts, Actions, and Experiences within the Human Affective Niche. En C. M. Worthman, C. A. Cummings, L. J. Kirmayer, R. Lemelson, y S. Kitayama (eds.), Culture, Mind, and Brain: Emerging Concepts, Models, and Applications (pp. 188-222). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108695374.010
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Nueva York: Houghton Mifflin.
Gómez-Robles, A., Hopkins, W. D., Schapiro, S. J. and Sherwood, C. C. (2015). Relaxed genetic control of cortical organization in human brains compared with chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(51), pp. 14799-14804. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1512646112
Gottlieb, G. (2003). On Making Behavioral Genetics Truly Developmental. Human Development, 46(6), pp. 337-355. https://doi.org/10.1159/000073306
Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10(1), pp. 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00556.x
Grimshaw, G. M.; Adelstein, A.; Bryden, M. P. and MacKinnon, G. E. (1998). First-language acquisition in adolescence: evidence for a critical period for verbal language development. Brain and Language, 63(2), pp. 237-255. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1997.1943
Guérard, K., Tremblay, S., y Saint-Aubin, J. (2009). The processing of spatial information in short-term memory: Insights from eye tracking the path length effect. Acta Psychologica, 132(2), pp. 136-144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.01.003
Guo, S., Sun, W., Chen, W., Zhang, J., y Liu, P. (2021). Impact of Artificial Elements on Mountain Landscape Perception: An Eye-Tracking Study. Land, 10(10), Article 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/land10101102
Gutchess, A. H., y Indeck, A. (2009). Cultural influences on memory. Progress in Brain Research, 178, pp. 137-150. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17809-3
Haidle, M. N., Bolus, M., Collard, M., Conard, N., Garofoli, D., Lombard, M., Nowell, A., Tennie, C., y Whiten, A. (2015). The Nature of Culture: An eight-grade model for the evolution and expansion of cultural capacities in hominins and other animals. Journal of Anthropological Sciences = Rivista Di Antropologia: JASS, 93, pp. 43-70. https://doi.org/10.4436/JASS.93011
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., y Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science, 19(1), pp. 12-17. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02038.x
Heidegger, M. (2003). Ser y tiempo. Madrid: Trotta.
Hodder, I. (1987). The archaeology of contextual meanings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hodder, I. (Ed.). (1989). The meanings of things: Material culture and symbolic expression. Londres: Harper Collins Academic.
Hodder, I. (2012). Archaeological Theory Today. Cambridge: Polity.
Holmes, N. P., y Spence, C. (2004). The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space. Cognitive processing, 5(2), pp. 94-105. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-004-0013-3
Hutchins, E. (2008). The role of cultural practices in the emergence of modern human intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1499), pp. 2011-2019. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0003
Hutchins, E. (2010). Cognitive Ecology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(4), pp. 705-715. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01089.x
Hutchins, E. (2014). The cultural ecosystem of human cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 27(1), pp. 34-49. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2013.830548
Ihde, D., y Malafouris, L. (2019). Homo faber Revisited: Postphenomenology and Material Engagement Theory. Philosophy y Technology, 32(2), pp. 195-214. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13347-018-0321-7
Iliopoulos, A. (2019). Material Engagement Theory and its philosophical ties to pragmatism. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), pp. 39-63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-018-9596-5
Iliopoulos, A., y Garofoli, D. (2016). The material dimensions of cognition: Reexamining the nature and emergence of the human mind. Quaternary International, 405, pp. 1-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2016.04.031
Jirak, D., Menz, M. M., Buccino, G., Borghi, A. M., y Binkofski, F. (2010). Grasping language—A short story on embodiment. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(3), pp. 711-720. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.020
Kandel, E. E., Schwartz, J. H. and Jessell, T. M. (1995). Essentials of neural science and behavior. Norwalk: CT. Appleton and Lange.
Kiverstein, J. (2018). Extended Cognition. En A. Newen, L. de Bruin, y S. Gallagher (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition (pp. 19-40). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.2
Krause, J., Lalueza-Fox, C., Orlando, L., Enard, W., Green, R. E., Burbano, H. A., Hublin, J.-J., Hänni, C., Fortea, J., de la Rasilla, M., Bertranpetit, J., Rosas, A., y Pääbo, S. (2007). The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals. Current Biology: CB, 17(21), pp. 1908-1912. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.008
Krueger, J. (2012). Seeing mind in action. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), pp. 149-173. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-011-9226-y
Krueger, J. (2016). Extended Mind and Religious Cognition. En N. Kasumi Clements (ed.), Religion: Mental Religion. Part of the Macmillan Interdisciplinary Handbooks: Religion series (pp. 237-254). Farmington Hills, Michigan: Macmillan Reference USA.
Laland, K. N. (2017). Darwin’s Unfinished Symphony: How Culture Made the Human Mind. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9781400884872
Latour, B. (2022). Nunca fuimos modernos. Ensayos de antropología simétrica. Madrid: Siglo XXI Editores.
Leiva, F. M., Méndez, J. H., Cabanillas, F. L., y Marchitto, M. (2016). Analysis of advertising effectiveness and usability in Travel 2.0 tools. An experimental study through eye-tracking technique. Tourism y Management Studies, 12(2), pp. 7-17.
Lombard, M. and Gärdenfors, P. (2017). Tracking the evolution of causal cognition in humans. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 95, pp. 1-16.
Lotem, A., Halpern, J. Y., Edelman, S. y Kolodny, O. (2017). «The evolution of cognitive mechanisms in response to cultural innovations». PNAS, 114(30), pp. 7915-7922. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620742114
Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement (1-1 online resource (xv, 304 pages). Cambridge Mass.: The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9476.001.0001
Malafouris, L. (2014). Creative thinging: The feeling of and for clay. Pragmatics y Cognition, 22(1), pp. 140-158. https://doi.org/10.1075/pc.22.1.08mal
Malafouris, L. (2015). Metaplasticity and the Primacy of Material Engagement. Time and Mind, 8(4), pp. 351-371. https://doi.org/10.1080/1751696X.2015.1111564
Malafouris, L. (2016). On Human Becoming and Incompleteness: A Material Engagement Approach to the Study of Embodiment in Evolution and Culture. En G. Etzelmüller y C. Tewes (eds.), Embodiment in Evolution and Culture (pp. 289-306). Aufl. Tübingen: Mohr Siebeck GmbH and Co. KG.
Malafouris, L. (2018). Bringing things to mind. 4Es and Material Engagement. En A. Newen, L. de Bruin, y S. Gallagher (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition (pp. 755-771). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.40
Malafouris, L. (2019). Mind and material engagement. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), pp. 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-018-9606-7
Malafouris, L., y Renfrew, C. (2010). The Cognitive Life of Things: Archaeology, Material Engagement and the Extended Mind. En L. Malafouris y C. Renfrew (eds.), The Cognitive Life of Things: Recasting the boundaries of the mind (pp. 1-12). Cambridge: McDonald Institute Monographs.
Maschner, H. (1996). The Politics of Settlement Choice on the Prehistoric Northwest Coast. En M. Aldenderfer y H. Maschner (eds.), Anthropology, Space, and Geographic Information Systems (pp. 175-189). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
McGann, M., De Jaegher, H., y Di Paolo, E. (2013). Enaction and Psychology. Review of General Psychology, 17(2), pp. 203-209. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032935
Menary, R. (2010a). Cognitive Integration and the Extended Mind. En R. Menary (ed.), The Extended Mind (pp. 226-243). Cambridge Mass.: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262014038.003.0010
Menary, R. (2010b). Introduction to the special issue on 4E cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), pp. 459-463. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-010-9187-6
Menary, R. (2010c). The Extended Mind. Cambridge Mass.: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262014038.001.0001
Menary, R. (2018). Cognitive Integration. How culture transforms us and extends our cognitive capabilities. En A. Newen, L. de Bruin, y S. Gallagher (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition (pp. 187-215). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.10
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1997). Fenomenología de la percepción. Madrid: Península.
Miłkowski, M., Krueger, J., Zawidzki, T., Wachowski, W. M., Clowes, R. W., Loughlin, V. K., y Hohol, M. (2018). From wide cognition to mechanisms: A silent revolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(2393), pp. 1-17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02393
Millán-Pascual, R., Martínez, L. M., Alonso-Pablos, D., Blanco, M. J., y Criado-Boado, F. (2021). Materialidades, espacio, pensamiento: Arqueología de la cognición visual. Trabajos de Prehistoria, 78(1), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.3989/tp.2021.12262
Miller, D. J., Duka, T., Stimpson, C. D., Schapiro, S. J., Baze. W. B., McArthur, M. J. and Herwood, C. C. (2012). Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109(41), pp. 16480-16485. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1117943109
Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind: A search for the origins of art, religion and science. Londres. Phoenix.
Muñoz Herrera, A. (2023). Paisaje, cognición y sacralidad en los espacios funerarios del antiguo Egipto (Tesis inédita de doctorado). Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid.
Müller, M. G., Kappas, A., y Olk, B. (2012). Perceiving press photography: A new integrative model, combining iconology with psychophysiological and eye-tracking methods. Visual Communication, 11(3), pp. 307-328. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470357212446410
Newen, A., Gallagher, S., y De Bruin, L. (2018). 4E Cognition: Historical Roots, Key Concepts, and Central Issues. En A. Newen, L. de Bruin, y S. Gallagher (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition (pp. 3-15). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.1
Nisbett, R. E., y Masuda, T. (2003). Culture and point of view. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(19), pp. 11163-11170. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1934527100
Norman, D. A. (1993). Things that make us smart: Defending human attributes in the age of the machine. Cambridge, Mass: Diversion Books.
Ooms, K., Andrienko, G., Andrienko, N., De Maeyer, P., y Fack, V. (2012). Analysing the spatial dimension of eye movement data using a visual analytic approach. Expert Systems with Applications, 39(1), pp. 1324-1332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eswa.2011.08.013
Pasqualini, I., Llobera, J., y Blanke, O. (2013). “Seeing” and “feeling” architecture: How bodily self-consciousness alters architectonic experience and affects the perception of interiors. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00354
Patalano, A. L., Juhasz, B. J., y Dicke, J. (2010). The relationship between indecisiveness and eye movement patterns in a decision making informational search task. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 23(4), pp. 353-368. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.661
Paulesu, E., McCrory, E., Fazio, F., Menoncello, L., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S. F., Cotelli, M., Cossu, G., Corte, F., Lorusso, M., Pesenti, S., Gallagher, A., Perani, D., Price, C., Frith, C. D., y Frith, U. (2000). A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neuroscience, 3(1), pp. 91-96. https://doi.org/10.1038/71163
Petanjek, Z.; Judaš, M., Šimić, G.; Rašin, M. R., Uylings, H. B. M.; Rakic, P., and Kostović, I. (2011). Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(32), pp. 13281–13286. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1105108108
Pulvermüller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(9), pp. 458-470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.004
Rakic, P. (1995). Evolution of neocortical parcellation: the perspective from experimental neuroembryology. En Changeux, J. P. and Chavaillon J. (eds.), Origins of the human brain (pp. 85-100). Oxford: Clarendon Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523901.003.0005
Renfrew, C. (2004). Towards a theory of material engagement. En E. DeMarrais, C. Gosden, y C. Renfrew (eds.), Rethinking Materiality: The Engagement of Mind with the Material World (pp. 23-32). Cambridge: McDonald Institute Monographs.
Renfrew, C. (2008). Neuroscience, evolution and the sapient paradox: the factuality of value and of the sacred. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 363, pp. 2041-2047. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0010
Renfrew, C. (2009). Prehistory: The making of the human mind. Londres: Modern Library.
Renfrew, C., y Zubrow, E. B. W. (1994). The Ancient Mind: Elements of Cognitive Archaeology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511598388
Richerson, P. J., y Boyd, R. (1978). A dual inheritance model of the human evolutionary process I: Basic postulates and a simple model. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 1(2), pp. 127-154. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-1750(78)80002-5
Rilling, J. K. and Insel, T. R. (1999). The primate neocórtex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Human Evolution, 37, pp. 191-223. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1999.0313
Rivera, A. y Menéndez, M. (2011). Las conductas simbólicas en el paleolítico. Un intento de comprensión y análisis desde el estructuralismo funcional. Espacio, Tiempo y Forma. Serie I Prehistoria y Arqueología. Nueva época, 4, pp. 11-42. DOI. https://doi.org/10.5944/etfi.4.2011.10739
Rivera, S. y Rivera, A. (2021). Origin and development of human cognition. Exaptation, coevolution and cognitive emergence. Cuadernos de Neuropsicología / Panamerican Journal of Neuropsychology, 15(1), pp. 186-198. DOI: 10.7714/CNPS/15.1.213
Rivera, A. y Menéndez, M. (2023). Manual de Arqueología y Paleoantropología cognitivas. Madrid: UNED.
Roepstorff, A. (2008). Things to think with: Words and objects as material symbols. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 363, pp. 2049-2054. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0015
Saito, K. (2022). La naturaleza contra el capital: el ecosocialismo en Karl Marx. Barcelona: Bellaterra.
Semendeferi, K., Lu, A., Schenker, N. and Damasio, H. (2002). Humans and great apes share a large frontal cortex. Nature neuroscience, 5(3), pp. 272-276. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn814
Shanks, M., y Tilley, C. (1987). Re-Constructing Archaeology: Theory and Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Shapiro, L. A. (2018). Flesh matters: The body in cognition. Mind Lang Mind and Language, 34(1), pp. 3-20. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12203
Silberstein, M., y Chemero, A. (2012). Complexity and extended phenomenological-cognitive systems. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(1), pp. 35-50. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2011.01168.x
Silva-Gago, M., Fedato, A., Hodgson, T., Terradillos-Bernal, M., Alonso-Alcalde, R., y Bruner, E. (2022). The Influence of Tool Morphology on Visual Attention During the Interaction with Lower Palaeolithic Stone Tools. Lithic Technology, 47(4), pp. 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1080/01977261.2022.2070335
Silva-Gago, M., Ioannidou, F., Fedato, A., Hodgson, T., y Bruner, E. (2022). Visual Attention and Cognitive Archaeology: An Eye-Tracking Study of Palaeolithic Stone Tools. Perception, 51, pp. 3-24. https://doi.org/10.1177/03010066211069504
Soliman, T., y Glenberg, A. M. (2014). The embodiment of culture. En L. Shapiro (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition (pp. 207-219). Londres: Routledge/Taylor y Francis Group.
Spaulding, A. C. (1962). Archaeological investigations on Agattu, Aleutian Islands. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan. https://doi.org/10.3998/mpub.11396747
Sprevak, M. (2009). Extended Cognition and Functionalism. The Journal of Philosophy, 106(9), pp. 503-527. https://doi.org/10.5840/jphil2009106937
Stuart-Fox, M., (2014). The origins of causal cognition in early hominins. Biology and Philosophy, 30(2), pp. 247-266. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-014-9462-y
Suchman, L. A. (1987). Plans and situated actions: The problem of human-machine communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sutton, J. (2010). Exograms and Interdisciplinarity: History, the Extended Mind, and the Civilizing Process. En R. Menary (ed.), The Extended Mind (pp. 189-225). Cambridge Mass.: mit Pres. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262014038.003.0009
Tabatabaeian, S. (2018). Eyes in the Dark: Using Eye-Tracking Technology to Investigate the Effects of Darkness on Human Cognition and Implications for Cave Archaeology [UC Merced]. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/65k1c6zr
Taylor, W. W. (1967). A study of archaeology. Southern Illinois U.P.
Thomas, J. (2006). Phenomenology and Material Culture. En C. Tilley, W. Keane, S. Küchler, M. Rowlands, y P. Spyer (eds.), Handbook of Material Culture (pp. 43-59). Londres: Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781848607972.n4
Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Cambridge, Mass.: The Belknap Press of Harvard University.
Tilley, C. (1994). A phenomenology of landscape: Places, paths, and monuments. Oxford: Berg.
Toni, I., de Lange, F. P., Noordzij, M. L., y Hagoort, P. (2008). Language beyond action. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1), pp. 71-79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.005
Tsai, J. L. (2007). Ideal Affect: Cultural Causes and Behavioral Consequences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), pp. 242-259. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00043.x
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., y Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/6730.001.0001
Varga, S., y Heck, D. H. (2017). Rhythms of the body, rhythms of the brain: Respiration, neural oscillations, and embodied cognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 56, pp. 77-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.09.008
Wang, P., Yang, W., Wang, D., y He, Y. (2021). Insights into Public Visual Behaviors through Eye-Tracking Tests: A Study Based on National Park System Pilot Area Landscapes. Land, 10(5), Article 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/land10050497
Westermann, G., Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M. W., y Thomas, M. S. C. (2007). Neuroconstructivism. Developmental Science, 10(1), pp. 75-83. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00567.x
Wheeler, M. (2017). The Revolution will not be Optimised: Radical Enactivism, Extended Functionalism and the Extensive Mind. Topoi, 36(3), pp. 457-472. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-015-9356-x
White, L. A. (1959). The Concept of Culture. American Anthropologist, 61(2), pp. 227-251. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1959.61.2.02a00040
Wynn, T., Coolidge, F., y Bright, M. (2009). Hohlenstein-Stadel and the Evolution of Human Conceptual Thought. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 19(1), pp. 73-84. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774309000043
Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, J., y Han, S. (2007). Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation. NeuroImage, 34(3), pp. 1310-1316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047
Zilhão, J., Angelucci, D. E., Badal-García, E., d’Errico, F., Daniel, F., Dayet, L., Douka, K., Higham, T. F. G., Martínez-Sánchez, M. J., Montes-Bernárdez, R., Murcia-Mascarós, S., Pérez-Sirvent, C., Roldán-García, C., Vanhaeren, M., Villaverde, V., Wood, R., y Zapata, J. (2010). Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(3), pp. 1023-1028. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914088107
Zubrow, E. B. W. (1994). Knowledge representation and archaeology: A cognitive example using GIS. En C. Renfrew y E. B. W. Zubrow (eds.), The Ancient Mind: Elements of Cognitive Archaeology (pp. 107-118). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511598388.012
Antonson, H., Mårdh, S., Wiklund, M., y Blomqvist, G. (2009). Effect of surrounding landscape on driving behaviour: A driving simulator study. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(4), pp. 493-502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.03.005
Arnheim, R. (2002). Arte y percepción visual: Psicología del ojo creador. Madrid: Alianza.
Baltes, P. B., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., y Rösler, F. (2006). Lifespan development and the brain: The perspective of biocultural co-constructivism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511499722
Barnard, P. J. (2010). From Executive Mechanisms Underlying Perception and Action to the Parallel Processing of Meaning. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S39-S54. https://doi.org/10.1086/650695
Barnard, P. J., Duke, D. J., Byrne, R. W., y Davidson, I. (2007). Differentiation in cognitive and emotional meanings: An evolutionary analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 21(6), 1155-1183. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701437477
Barona, A. M., y Malafouris, L. (2021). On making futures with human touch. Adaptive Behavior, 30(6), 551-553. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059712321989428
Barrett, J. C. (2012). Agency: A revisionist account. En I. Hodder (ed.), Archaeological Theory Today (pp. 146-166). Cambridge: Polity.
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded Cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 617-645. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
Bateson, G. (2008). Steps to an ecology of mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Beja-Pereira, A., Luikart, G., England, P. R., Bradley, D. G., Jann, O. C., Bertorelle, G., Chamberlain, A. T., Nunes, T. P., Metodiev, S., Ferrand, N., y Erhardt, G. (2003). Gene-culture coevolution between cattle milk protein genes and human lactase genes. Nature Genetics, 35(4), 311-313. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng1263
Benazzi, S., Douka, K., Fornai, C., Bauer, C. C., Kullmer, O., Svoboda, J., Pap, I., Mallegni, F., Bayle, P., Coquerelle, M., Condemi, S., Ronchitelli, A., Harvati, K., y Weber, G. W. (2011). Early dispersal of modern humans in Europe and implications for Neanderthal behaviour. Nature, 479(7374), Article 7374. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10617
Bender, A. (2019). The role of culture and evolution for human cognition. Top. Cogn. Sci. 12 (4), pp. 1403-1420. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12449
Bender, A. (2020). What Is Causal Cognition?. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00003
Bender, A., Beller, S. y Medin, D. L. (2017). Causal Cognition and Culture. En Michael R. Waldmann (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Causal Reasoning (pp. 717-738). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199399550.013.34
Bercury, K. K. and Macklin, W. B. (2015). Dynamics and Mechanisms of CNS Myelination. Developmental Cell, 32(4), pp. 447-458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.devcel.2015.01.016
Berto, R., Massaccesi, S., y Pasini, M. (2008). Do eye movements measured across high and low fascination photographs differ? Addressing Kaplan’s fascination hypothesis. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(2), pp. 185-191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.11.004
Białowąs, S., y Szyszka, A. (2019). Eye-tracking in Marketing Research. En R. Romanowski (ed.), Managing Economic Innovations – Methods and Instruments (pp. 91-104). Poznań: Bogucki Wyd. https://doi.org/10.12657/9788379862771-6
Binford, S. R., y Binford, L. R. (1968). New perspectives in archeology. Chicago: Aldine Pub. Co.
Bjorklund, D., y Sellers, P. (2014). Memory Development in Evolutionary Perspective. En P. J. Bauer y R. Fivush (eds.), The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory (Vol.1, pp. 126-156). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118597705.ch7
Boivin, N. (2004). Mind over matter? Collapsing the mind-matter dichotomy in material culture studies. En E. DeMarris, C. Gosden, y C. Renfrew (eds.), Rethinking materiality: The engagement of mind with the material world (pp. 63-71). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.
Bruner, E. (2010). Morphological Differences in the Parietal Lobes within the Human Genus: A Neurofunctional Perspective. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S77-S88. https://doi.org/10.1086/650729
Byrge, L., Sporns, O., y Smith, L. B. (2014). Developmental process emerges from extended brain-body-behavior networks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), pp. 395-403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.010
Campanaro, D. M., y Landeschi, G. (2022). Re-viewing Pompeian domestic space through combined virtual reality-based eye tracking and 3D GIS. Antiquity, 96 (386), pp. 479-486. https://doi.org/10.15184/aqy.2022.12
Chen, C., Burton, M., Greenberger, E., y Dmitrieva, J. (1999). Population Migration and the Variation of Dopamine D4 Receptor (DRD4) Allele Frequencies Around the Globe. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(5), pp. 309-324. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00015-X
Chiao, J. Y., y Blizinsky, K. D. (2010). Culture-gene coevolution of individualism-collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 277(1681), pp. 529-537. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1650
Chiao, J. Y., Cheon, B. K., Pornpattanangkul, N., Mrazek, A. J., y Blizinsky, K. D. (2013). Cultural Neuroscience: Progress and Promise. Psychological inquiry, 24(1), pp. 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2013.752715
Chiao, J. Y., Harada, T., Oby, E. R., Li, Z., Parrish, T., y Bridge, D. J. (2009). Neural representations of social status hierarchy in human inferior parietal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), pp. 354-363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.023
Chiao, J. Y., Hariri, A. R., Harada, T., Mano, Y., Sadato, N., Parrish, T. B., y Iidaka, T. (2010). Theory and methods in cultural neuroscience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2-3), pp. 356-361. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq063
Chiao, J. Y., Iidaka, T., Gordon, H. L., Nogawa, J., Bar, M., Aminoff, E., Sadato, N., y Ambady, N. (2008). Cultural specificity in amygdala response to fear faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(12), pp. 2167-2174. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20151
Chiao, J. Y., Zhang, L., y Sadato, N. (2022). Culture and Genomics. En J. Y. Chiao, S. C. Li, R. Turner, S. Y. Lee-Tauler, y B. Pringle (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cultural Neuroscience and Global Mental Health (pp. 144-155). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190057695.013.7
Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E., y Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(35), pp. 12629-12633. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506162102
Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/1552.001.0001
Clark, A. (2006). Material symbols. Philosophical Psychology, 19(3), pp. 291-307. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515080600689872
Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195333213.001.0001
Clark, A. (2010). Material surrogacy and the Supernatural: Reflections on the Role of Artefacts in «Off-line» cognition. En L. Malafouris, C. Renfrew, y McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research (eds.), The cognitive life of things: Recasting the boundaries of the mind (pp. 23-28). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research ; Distributed by Oxbow Books ; USA [distributor], David Brown Co.
Clark, A. (2023). The experience machine: How our Minds Predict and Shape Reality. Londres: Allen Lane.
Clark, A., y Chalmers, D. (1998). The Extended Mind. Analysis, 58(1), pp. 7-19. https://doi.org/10.1093/analys/58.1.7
Clarke, D. L. (1968). Analytical archaeology. Londres: Methuen.
Colagè, I. y d’Errico, F. (2018). Culture: The Driving Force of Human Cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(12), pp. 1-19. En Bender, A., Beller, S. and Jordan, F. (Topic Editors). The Cultural Evolution of Cognition. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12372
Cole, M. (1974). The Cultural context of learning and thinking: An exploration in experimental anthropology. Londres: Tavistock Pub.
Constant, A., Tschantz, A. D. D., Millidge, B., Criado-Boado, F., Martinez, L. M., Müeller, J., y Clark, A. (2021). The acquisition of culturally patterned attention styles under active inference. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 15, a729665. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbot.2021.729665
Cook, A. (2018). 4E cognition and the humanities. En A. Newen, L. de Bruin, y S. Gallagher (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition (pp. 875-890). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.47
Coolidge, F. L., y Wynn, T. (2005). Working Memory, its Executive Functions, and the Emergence of Modern Thinking. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 15(1), pp. 5-26. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774305000016
Costello, M. C., y Bloesch, E. K. (2017). Are Older Adults Less Embodied? A Review of Age Effects through the Lens of Embodied Cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00267
Criado Boado, F., Alonso-Pablos, D., Blanco, M. J., Porto, Y., Rodríguez-Paz, A., Cabrejas, E., del Barrio-Álvarez, E., y Martínez, L. M. (2019). Coevolution of visual behaviour, the material world and social complexity, depicted by the eye-tracking of archaeological objects in humans. Scientific Reports, 9(1), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-39661-w
Criado-Boado, F., Martínez, L. M., Blanco, M. J., Alonso-Pablos, D., y Verdonkschot, J. (2024). Archaeologiques of sight: The visual world fosters the engagement between doing, seeing, and thinking. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 73, 101568. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2023.101568
Damasio, A. R. (2000). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Londres: William Heinemann.
Davidson, I., y Noble, W. (1989). The Archaeology of Perception: Traces of Depiction and Language [and Comments and Reply]. Current Anthropology, 30(2), pp. 125-155. https://doi.org/10.1086/203723
De Lucio, J. V., Mohamadian, M., Ruiz, J. P., Banayas, J., y Bernaldez, F. G. (1996). Visual landscape exploration as revealed by eye movement tracking. Landscape and Urban Planning, 34(2), pp. 135-142. https://doi.org/10.1016/0169-2046(95)00208-1
Dehaene, S. y Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56, pp. 384-398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.004
D’Errico, F. (2003). The invisible frontier. A multiple species model for the origin of behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(4), pp. 188-202. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.10113
D’Errico, F., y Stringer, C. B. (2011). Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), pp. 1060-1069. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0340
Descola, P. (2013). Más allá de naturaleza y cultura. Buenos Aires: Amorrortu.
Domínguez, J. F., Lewis, E. D., Turner, R., y Egan, G. F. (2009). The brain in culture and culture in the brain: A review of core issues in neuroanthropology. Progress in Brain Research, 178, pp. 43-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17804-4
Domínguez, J. F., Turner, R., Lewis, E. D., y Egan, G. (2010). Neuroanthropology: A humanistic science for the study of the culture-brain nexus. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2-3), pp. 138-147. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp024
Donald, M. (1998). Material Culture and Cognition: Concluding Thoughts. En C. Renfrew y C. Scarre (eds.), Cognition and Material Culture: The Archaeology of Symbolic Storage (pp. 181-187). Cambridge: McDonald Institute Monographs.
Dupont, L., Antrop, M., y Van Eetvelde, V. (2013). Eye-tracking Analysis in Landscape Perception Research: Influence of Photograph Properties and Landscape Characteristics. Landscape Research, 39(4), pp. 417-432. https://doi.org/10.1080/01426397.2013.773966
Elfenbein, H. A., y Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), pp. 203-235. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203
Flor, N., y Hutchins, E. (1991). Analyzing distributed cognition in software teams: A case study of team programming during perfective software maintenance. En J. Koenemann-Belliveau, T. G. Moher, y S. Robertson (eds.), Proceedings of the Fourth Annual Workshop on empirical studies of programmers (pp. 36-59). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.
Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/0199271941.001.0001
Gallagher, S. (2013). The socially extended mind. Cognitive Systems Research, 25-26, pp. 4-12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2013.03.008
Gallagher, S. (2018). Situating Interaction in Peripersonal and Extrapersonal Space: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives. En T. Hünefeldt y A. Schlitte (eds.), Situatedness and Place: Multidisciplinary Perspectives on the Spatio-temporal Contingency of Human Life (pp. 67-79). Cham: Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-92937-8_5
García-Porrero, J. A. (1999). Evolución del cerebro: la génesis de la mente. In García-Porrero, J. A. (ed.), Genes, cultura y mente: una reflexión multidisciplinar sobre la naturaleza humana en la década del cerebro (pp. 129-158). Santander: Servicio de publicaciones de la Universidad de Cantabria.
Garofoli, D. (2015). Cognitive archaeology without behavioral modernity: An eliminativist attempt. Quaternary International, 405, pp. 125-135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.06.061
Garofoli, D. (2016). Metaplasticit-ies: Material Engagement Meets Mutational Enhancement. En G. Etzelmüller y C. Tewes (Eds.), Embodiment in Evolution and Culture (pp. 307-336). Aufl. Tubingen: Mohr Siebeck GmbH and Co. KG. https://www.jstor.org/stable/j.ctt2250vc6.22
Garofoli, D., y Haidle, M. N. (2014). Epistemological problems in Cognitive Archaeology: An anti-relativistic proposal towards methodological uniformity. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 92, pp. 7-41. https://doi.org/10.4436/jass.91003
Gendron, M., Mesquita, B., y Barrett, L. F. (2020). The Brain as a Cultural Artifact: Concepts, Actions, and Experiences within the Human Affective Niche. En C. M. Worthman, C. A. Cummings, L. J. Kirmayer, R. Lemelson, y S. Kitayama (eds.), Culture, Mind, and Brain: Emerging Concepts, Models, and Applications (pp. 188-222). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108695374.010
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Nueva York: Houghton Mifflin.
Gómez-Robles, A., Hopkins, W. D., Schapiro, S. J. and Sherwood, C. C. (2015). Relaxed genetic control of cortical organization in human brains compared with chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(51), pp. 14799-14804. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1512646112
Gottlieb, G. (2003). On Making Behavioral Genetics Truly Developmental. Human Development, 46(6), pp. 337-355. https://doi.org/10.1159/000073306
Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10(1), pp. 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00556.x
Grimshaw, G. M.; Adelstein, A.; Bryden, M. P. and MacKinnon, G. E. (1998). First-language acquisition in adolescence: evidence for a critical period for verbal language development. Brain and Language, 63(2), pp. 237-255. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1997.1943
Guérard, K., Tremblay, S., y Saint-Aubin, J. (2009). The processing of spatial information in short-term memory: Insights from eye tracking the path length effect. Acta Psychologica, 132(2), pp. 136-144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.01.003
Guo, S., Sun, W., Chen, W., Zhang, J., y Liu, P. (2021). Impact of Artificial Elements on Mountain Landscape Perception: An Eye-Tracking Study. Land, 10(10), Article 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/land10101102
Gutchess, A. H., y Indeck, A. (2009). Cultural influences on memory. Progress in Brain Research, 178, pp. 137-150. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17809-3
Haidle, M. N., Bolus, M., Collard, M., Conard, N., Garofoli, D., Lombard, M., Nowell, A., Tennie, C., y Whiten, A. (2015). The Nature of Culture: An eight-grade model for the evolution and expansion of cultural capacities in hominins and other animals. Journal of Anthropological Sciences = Rivista Di Antropologia: JASS, 93, pp. 43-70. https://doi.org/10.4436/JASS.93011
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., y Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science, 19(1), pp. 12-17. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02038.x
Heidegger, M. (2003). Ser y tiempo. Madrid: Trotta.
Hodder, I. (1987). The archaeology of contextual meanings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hodder, I. (Ed.). (1989). The meanings of things: Material culture and symbolic expression. Londres: Harper Collins Academic.
Hodder, I. (2012). Archaeological Theory Today. Cambridge: Polity.
Holmes, N. P., y Spence, C. (2004). The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space. Cognitive processing, 5(2), pp. 94-105. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-004-0013-3
Hutchins, E. (2008). The role of cultural practices in the emergence of modern human intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1499), pp. 2011-2019. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0003
Hutchins, E. (2010). Cognitive Ecology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(4), pp. 705-715. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01089.x
Hutchins, E. (2014). The cultural ecosystem of human cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 27(1), pp. 34-49. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2013.830548
Ihde, D., y Malafouris, L. (2019). Homo faber Revisited: Postphenomenology and Material Engagement Theory. Philosophy y Technology, 32(2), pp. 195-214. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13347-018-0321-7
Iliopoulos, A. (2019). Material Engagement Theory and its philosophical ties to pragmatism. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), pp. 39-63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-018-9596-5
Iliopoulos, A., y Garofoli, D. (2016). The material dimensions of cognition: Reexamining the nature and emergence of the human mind. Quaternary International, 405, pp. 1-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2016.04.031
Jirak, D., Menz, M. M., Buccino, G., Borghi, A. M., y Binkofski, F. (2010). Grasping language—A short story on embodiment. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(3), pp. 711-720. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.020
Kandel, E. E., Schwartz, J. H. and Jessell, T. M. (1995). Essentials of neural science and behavior. Norwalk: CT. Appleton and Lange.
Kiverstein, J. (2018). Extended Cognition. En A. Newen, L. de Bruin, y S. Gallagher (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition (pp. 19-40). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.2
Krause, J., Lalueza-Fox, C., Orlando, L., Enard, W., Green, R. E., Burbano, H. A., Hublin, J.-J., Hänni, C., Fortea, J., de la Rasilla, M., Bertranpetit, J., Rosas, A., y Pääbo, S. (2007). The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals. Current Biology: CB, 17(21), pp. 1908-1912. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.008
Krueger, J. (2012). Seeing mind in action. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), pp. 149-173. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-011-9226-y
Krueger, J. (2016). Extended Mind and Religious Cognition. En N. Kasumi Clements (ed.), Religion: Mental Religion. Part of the Macmillan Interdisciplinary Handbooks: Religion series (pp. 237-254). Farmington Hills, Michigan: Macmillan Reference USA.
Laland, K. N. (2017). Darwin’s Unfinished Symphony: How Culture Made the Human Mind. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9781400884872
Latour, B. (2022). Nunca fuimos modernos. Ensayos de antropología simétrica. Madrid: Siglo XXI Editores.
Leiva, F. M., Méndez, J. H., Cabanillas, F. L., y Marchitto, M. (2016). Analysis of advertising effectiveness and usability in Travel 2.0 tools. An experimental study through eye-tracking technique. Tourism y Management Studies, 12(2), pp. 7-17.
Lombard, M. and Gärdenfors, P. (2017). Tracking the evolution of causal cognition in humans. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 95, pp. 1-16.
Lotem, A., Halpern, J. Y., Edelman, S. y Kolodny, O. (2017). «The evolution of cognitive mechanisms in response to cultural innovations». PNAS, 114(30), pp. 7915-7922. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620742114
Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement (1-1 online resource (xv, 304 pages). Cambridge Mass.: The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9476.001.0001
Malafouris, L. (2014). Creative thinging: The feeling of and for clay. Pragmatics y Cognition, 22(1), pp. 140-158. https://doi.org/10.1075/pc.22.1.08mal
Malafouris, L. (2015). Metaplasticity and the Primacy of Material Engagement. Time and Mind, 8(4), pp. 351-371. https://doi.org/10.1080/1751696X.2015.1111564
Malafouris, L. (2016). On Human Becoming and Incompleteness: A Material Engagement Approach to the Study of Embodiment in Evolution and Culture. En G. Etzelmüller y C. Tewes (eds.), Embodiment in Evolution and Culture (pp. 289-306). Aufl. Tübingen: Mohr Siebeck GmbH and Co. KG.
Malafouris, L. (2018). Bringing things to mind. 4Es and Material Engagement. En A. Newen, L. de Bruin, y S. Gallagher (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition (pp. 755-771). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.40
Malafouris, L. (2019). Mind and material engagement. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), pp. 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-018-9606-7
Malafouris, L., y Renfrew, C. (2010). The Cognitive Life of Things: Archaeology, Material Engagement and the Extended Mind. En L. Malafouris y C. Renfrew (eds.), The Cognitive Life of Things: Recasting the boundaries of the mind (pp. 1-12). Cambridge: McDonald Institute Monographs.
Maschner, H. (1996). The Politics of Settlement Choice on the Prehistoric Northwest Coast. En M. Aldenderfer y H. Maschner (eds.), Anthropology, Space, and Geographic Information Systems (pp. 175-189). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
McGann, M., De Jaegher, H., y Di Paolo, E. (2013). Enaction and Psychology. Review of General Psychology, 17(2), pp. 203-209. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032935
Menary, R. (2010a). Cognitive Integration and the Extended Mind. En R. Menary (ed.), The Extended Mind (pp. 226-243). Cambridge Mass.: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262014038.003.0010
Menary, R. (2010b). Introduction to the special issue on 4E cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), pp. 459-463. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-010-9187-6
Menary, R. (2010c). The Extended Mind. Cambridge Mass.: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262014038.001.0001
Menary, R. (2018). Cognitive Integration. How culture transforms us and extends our cognitive capabilities. En A. Newen, L. de Bruin, y S. Gallagher (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition (pp. 187-215). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.10
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1997). Fenomenología de la percepción. Madrid: Península.
Miłkowski, M., Krueger, J., Zawidzki, T., Wachowski, W. M., Clowes, R. W., Loughlin, V. K., y Hohol, M. (2018). From wide cognition to mechanisms: A silent revolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(2393), pp. 1-17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02393
Millán-Pascual, R., Martínez, L. M., Alonso-Pablos, D., Blanco, M. J., y Criado-Boado, F. (2021). Materialidades, espacio, pensamiento: Arqueología de la cognición visual. Trabajos de Prehistoria, 78(1), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.3989/tp.2021.12262
Miller, D. J., Duka, T., Stimpson, C. D., Schapiro, S. J., Baze. W. B., McArthur, M. J. and Herwood, C. C. (2012). Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109(41), pp. 16480-16485. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1117943109
Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind: A search for the origins of art, religion and science. Londres. Phoenix.
Muñoz Herrera, A. (2023). Paisaje, cognición y sacralidad en los espacios funerarios del antiguo Egipto (Tesis inédita de doctorado). Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid.
Müller, M. G., Kappas, A., y Olk, B. (2012). Perceiving press photography: A new integrative model, combining iconology with psychophysiological and eye-tracking methods. Visual Communication, 11(3), pp. 307-328. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470357212446410
Newen, A., Gallagher, S., y De Bruin, L. (2018). 4E Cognition: Historical Roots, Key Concepts, and Central Issues. En A. Newen, L. de Bruin, y S. Gallagher (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition (pp. 3-15). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.013.1
Nisbett, R. E., y Masuda, T. (2003). Culture and point of view. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(19), pp. 11163-11170. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1934527100
Norman, D. A. (1993). Things that make us smart: Defending human attributes in the age of the machine. Cambridge, Mass: Diversion Books.
Ooms, K., Andrienko, G., Andrienko, N., De Maeyer, P., y Fack, V. (2012). Analysing the spatial dimension of eye movement data using a visual analytic approach. Expert Systems with Applications, 39(1), pp. 1324-1332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eswa.2011.08.013
Pasqualini, I., Llobera, J., y Blanke, O. (2013). “Seeing” and “feeling” architecture: How bodily self-consciousness alters architectonic experience and affects the perception of interiors. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00354
Patalano, A. L., Juhasz, B. J., y Dicke, J. (2010). The relationship between indecisiveness and eye movement patterns in a decision making informational search task. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 23(4), pp. 353-368. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.661
Paulesu, E., McCrory, E., Fazio, F., Menoncello, L., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S. F., Cotelli, M., Cossu, G., Corte, F., Lorusso, M., Pesenti, S., Gallagher, A., Perani, D., Price, C., Frith, C. D., y Frith, U. (2000). A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neuroscience, 3(1), pp. 91-96. https://doi.org/10.1038/71163
Petanjek, Z.; Judaš, M., Šimić, G.; Rašin, M. R., Uylings, H. B. M.; Rakic, P., and Kostović, I. (2011). Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(32), pp. 13281–13286. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1105108108
Pulvermüller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(9), pp. 458-470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.004
Rakic, P. (1995). Evolution of neocortical parcellation: the perspective from experimental neuroembryology. En Changeux, J. P. and Chavaillon J. (eds.), Origins of the human brain (pp. 85-100). Oxford: Clarendon Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523901.003.0005
Renfrew, C. (2004). Towards a theory of material engagement. En E. DeMarrais, C. Gosden, y C. Renfrew (eds.), Rethinking Materiality: The Engagement of Mind with the Material World (pp. 23-32). Cambridge: McDonald Institute Monographs.
Renfrew, C. (2008). Neuroscience, evolution and the sapient paradox: the factuality of value and of the sacred. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 363, pp. 2041-2047. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0010
Renfrew, C. (2009). Prehistory: The making of the human mind. Londres: Modern Library.
Renfrew, C., y Zubrow, E. B. W. (1994). The Ancient Mind: Elements of Cognitive Archaeology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511598388
Richerson, P. J., y Boyd, R. (1978). A dual inheritance model of the human evolutionary process I: Basic postulates and a simple model. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 1(2), pp. 127-154. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-1750(78)80002-5
Rilling, J. K. and Insel, T. R. (1999). The primate neocórtex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Human Evolution, 37, pp. 191-223. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1999.0313
Rivera, A. y Menéndez, M. (2011). Las conductas simbólicas en el paleolítico. Un intento de comprensión y análisis desde el estructuralismo funcional. Espacio, Tiempo y Forma. Serie I Prehistoria y Arqueología. Nueva época, 4, pp. 11-42. DOI. https://doi.org/10.5944/etfi.4.2011.10739
Rivera, S. y Rivera, A. (2021). Origin and development of human cognition. Exaptation, coevolution and cognitive emergence. Cuadernos de Neuropsicología / Panamerican Journal of Neuropsychology, 15(1), pp. 186-198. DOI: 10.7714/CNPS/15.1.213
Rivera, A. y Menéndez, M. (2023). Manual de Arqueología y Paleoantropología cognitivas. Madrid: UNED.
Roepstorff, A. (2008). Things to think with: Words and objects as material symbols. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 363, pp. 2049-2054. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0015
Saito, K. (2022). La naturaleza contra el capital: el ecosocialismo en Karl Marx. Barcelona: Bellaterra.
Semendeferi, K., Lu, A., Schenker, N. and Damasio, H. (2002). Humans and great apes share a large frontal cortex. Nature neuroscience, 5(3), pp. 272-276. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn814
Shanks, M., y Tilley, C. (1987). Re-Constructing Archaeology: Theory and Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Shapiro, L. A. (2018). Flesh matters: The body in cognition. Mind Lang Mind and Language, 34(1), pp. 3-20. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12203
Silberstein, M., y Chemero, A. (2012). Complexity and extended phenomenological-cognitive systems. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(1), pp. 35-50. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2011.01168.x
Silva-Gago, M., Fedato, A., Hodgson, T., Terradillos-Bernal, M., Alonso-Alcalde, R., y Bruner, E. (2022). The Influence of Tool Morphology on Visual Attention During the Interaction with Lower Palaeolithic Stone Tools. Lithic Technology, 47(4), pp. 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1080/01977261.2022.2070335
Silva-Gago, M., Ioannidou, F., Fedato, A., Hodgson, T., y Bruner, E. (2022). Visual Attention and Cognitive Archaeology: An Eye-Tracking Study of Palaeolithic Stone Tools. Perception, 51, pp. 3-24. https://doi.org/10.1177/03010066211069504
Soliman, T., y Glenberg, A. M. (2014). The embodiment of culture. En L. Shapiro (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition (pp. 207-219). Londres: Routledge/Taylor y Francis Group.
Spaulding, A. C. (1962). Archaeological investigations on Agattu, Aleutian Islands. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan. https://doi.org/10.3998/mpub.11396747
Sprevak, M. (2009). Extended Cognition and Functionalism. The Journal of Philosophy, 106(9), pp. 503-527. https://doi.org/10.5840/jphil2009106937
Stuart-Fox, M., (2014). The origins of causal cognition in early hominins. Biology and Philosophy, 30(2), pp. 247-266. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-014-9462-y
Suchman, L. A. (1987). Plans and situated actions: The problem of human-machine communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sutton, J. (2010). Exograms and Interdisciplinarity: History, the Extended Mind, and the Civilizing Process. En R. Menary (ed.), The Extended Mind (pp. 189-225). Cambridge Mass.: mit Pres. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262014038.003.0009
Tabatabaeian, S. (2018). Eyes in the Dark: Using Eye-Tracking Technology to Investigate the Effects of Darkness on Human Cognition and Implications for Cave Archaeology [UC Merced]. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/65k1c6zr
Taylor, W. W. (1967). A study of archaeology. Southern Illinois U.P.
Thomas, J. (2006). Phenomenology and Material Culture. En C. Tilley, W. Keane, S. Küchler, M. Rowlands, y P. Spyer (eds.), Handbook of Material Culture (pp. 43-59). Londres: Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781848607972.n4
Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Cambridge, Mass.: The Belknap Press of Harvard University.
Tilley, C. (1994). A phenomenology of landscape: Places, paths, and monuments. Oxford: Berg.
Toni, I., de Lange, F. P., Noordzij, M. L., y Hagoort, P. (2008). Language beyond action. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1), pp. 71-79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.005
Tsai, J. L. (2007). Ideal Affect: Cultural Causes and Behavioral Consequences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), pp. 242-259. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00043.x
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., y Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/6730.001.0001
Varga, S., y Heck, D. H. (2017). Rhythms of the body, rhythms of the brain: Respiration, neural oscillations, and embodied cognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 56, pp. 77-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.09.008
Wang, P., Yang, W., Wang, D., y He, Y. (2021). Insights into Public Visual Behaviors through Eye-Tracking Tests: A Study Based on National Park System Pilot Area Landscapes. Land, 10(5), Article 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/land10050497
Westermann, G., Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M. W., y Thomas, M. S. C. (2007). Neuroconstructivism. Developmental Science, 10(1), pp. 75-83. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00567.x
Wheeler, M. (2017). The Revolution will not be Optimised: Radical Enactivism, Extended Functionalism and the Extensive Mind. Topoi, 36(3), pp. 457-472. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-015-9356-x
White, L. A. (1959). The Concept of Culture. American Anthropologist, 61(2), pp. 227-251. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1959.61.2.02a00040
Wynn, T., Coolidge, F., y Bright, M. (2009). Hohlenstein-Stadel and the Evolution of Human Conceptual Thought. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 19(1), pp. 73-84. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774309000043
Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, J., y Han, S. (2007). Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation. NeuroImage, 34(3), pp. 1310-1316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047
Zilhão, J., Angelucci, D. E., Badal-García, E., d’Errico, F., Daniel, F., Dayet, L., Douka, K., Higham, T. F. G., Martínez-Sánchez, M. J., Montes-Bernárdez, R., Murcia-Mascarós, S., Pérez-Sirvent, C., Roldán-García, C., Vanhaeren, M., Villaverde, V., Wood, R., y Zapata, J. (2010). Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(3), pp. 1023-1028. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914088107
Zubrow, E. B. W. (1994). Knowledge representation and archaeology: A cognitive example using GIS. En C. Renfrew y E. B. W. Zubrow (eds.), The Ancient Mind: Elements of Cognitive Archaeology (pp. 107-118). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511598388.012
Muñoz Herrera, A. (2024). Sobre la reciprocidad de naturaleza y cultura. La Cognición 4E y su perspectiva arqueológica en el paisaje. El Futuro Del Pasado, 16, 455–502. https://doi.org/10.14201/fdp.31611
Descargas
Los datos de descargas todavía no están disponibles.
+
−